Case role
Case roles, according to the work by Fillmore, are the semantic roles of noun phrases in relation to the syntactic structures that contain these noun phrases. The term case role is most widely used for purely semantic relations, including theta roles and thematic roles, that can be independent of the morpho-syntax. The concept of case roles is related to the larger notion of Case which is defined as a system of marking dependent nouns for the type of semantic or syntactic relationship they bear to their heads. Case traditionally refers to inflectional marking.
The relationships between nouns and their containing structures is one of both syntactic and semantic value. The syntactic positional relationships between forms in sentences varies cross-linguistically and allows grammarians to observe semantic values in these nouns by examining their syntactic values. Using these semantic values gives the base for considering case role in a specific language.
Case theory includes, in addition to its inventory of structural cases, a series of lexical cases that are assigned at deep-structure in conjunction with theta role assignment.
In addition to its relation to Case, these semantic notions of case role are closely related to morphological case as well.
Inventory of case roles
The following list of case roles are frequently distinguished in literature, but are by no means an exhaustive list since there is no consensus on the universal inventory of roles, nor a universal agreement as to the correct assignment of constituents to roles.Case Role | Description | Example |
Patient | Fillmore refers to this case role as 'object' or 'objective' and can be described in three ways:
| 1. The sky is blue. 2. The lion is in the cave. 3. The bird ate the worm. |
Agent | An entity that performs an activity or brings about a change of state | The robots assembled the car. |
Instrument | The means by which an activity or change of state is carried out | She squashed the spider with a slipper. |
Experiencer | The creature experiencing an emotion or perception | They love music. |
Location | The position of an entity, referring to both the temporal and spatial roles | The vase is on the table. or Canada Day is on a Tuesday. |
Source | The point from which an entity moves or derives | They got news from home. |
Destination | The point to or towards which an entity moves or is oriented | He turned to the altar and walked towards it. |
Recipient | A sentient destination | She gave her spare change to the collectors. |
Purpose | The purpose of an activity | He went to the Red Rooster for some take-out. |
Beneficiary | The animate entity on whose behalf an activity is carried out | She did the shopping for her mother. |
Manner | The way in which an activity is done or the way in which a change of state takes place | He did it with great skill. |
Extent | The distance, area or time over which an activity is carried out or over which a state holds | It lasted the winter. |
Possessor | The entity that possesses another entity | I saw John's golf clubs. |
Early contributions to case role
Roman Jakobson's work on Case Role in RussianIn his article on the case system of Russian, Roman Jakobson closely examines case assignment and argues for a feature decomposition of case on the basis of semantic considerations. Jakobson proposed a 3-feature binary case system for Russian case which includes the following: . The term 'marginal', distinguishes the direct and non-direct cases. Only the cases may occur in subject and object position. 'Quantifying' indicates the relevance of the extent to which the noun is a participant in the event. 'Ascriptive' puts emphasis on directionality.
Jakobson considers case to be bundles of these three features which can be assigned to morphological cases in the following way:
Case | Marginal | Quantifying | Ascriptive | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
Nominative | He uses these decomposed case features to account for the case alternations in subject and object position. Additionally, he also argues for how there should be a universal inventory of case roles. Fillmore's work on case role closely follows up on these ideas and contributions by Jakobson. Multiple case rolesThere is a theory that multiple case roles can be assigned to noun phrases. The reasons for having more than one case role is due to the differences in the sentences’ semantic effects. Bhat proposed that the speaker of a language would have the option of assigning any single case role out of the multiple case role alternations available in a given context. This is not done by a transformational rule, but due to the deep structure representations.Examples || John sprayed the wall with paint. ||John sprayed paint on the wall. In comparing sentences and, it demonstrates that the surface structure representation for each sentence is different. However, in order to account for the meaning distinctions that exist in and, it has been demonstrated that these distinctions are due to the differences in the deep structures of each sentence and can be resolved by assigning a different case role to the NP. With "paint" acting as an instrument, and "the wall" being the location, sentence might infer that all the paint was used, but that not necessarily all the wall was covered. Sentence might imply that the whole wall is covered, but that John did not use all the paint he had available to him. This analysis supports how NPs can have multiple case roles. Fillmore's alternative theory Conversely, it has been suggested by Fillmore that in the case of sentences following the structure of the above examples, the noun phrases are not assigned multiple case roles, but instead retain the same case roles in both sentences. The difference in meaning, then, is attributed to a transformation that takes both identical deep structures and chooses the direct object as it appears in the surface form. This allows for the basic similarity in the sentence pairs, as the event taking place within them is essentially the same. What differs is the surface structure's semantic meaning, which while related to the case role of the noun phrases, can be proven as a result of a transformation that occurred between the deep structure and surface structure. Both theories account for semantic meaning, with Bhat allowing for nouns to contain multiple semantic meanings through multiple case roles, and Fillmore maintaining the single case role assignment theory but re-introducing semantic transformations. Examples in Kannada Kannada affords some good evidence of how multiple case roles can be assigned to NPs in the following two examples: how NPs can be assigned either Object or Location case roles, and how NPs can be assigned either Agent or Experiencer case roles. Evidence for multiple case roles demonstrated in Kannada: NPs can be assigned either Object or Location case roles. Evidence is found from the meaning distinctions of exhaustiveness: Examples ||bekku maravannu hattitu cat tree climbed "The cat climbed the tree." || bekku marakke hattitu cat tree climbed "The cat climbed to the tree." implies that the cat climbed the tree from the ground itself, whereas has no such implication. The common feature of these two uses is that whenever an element occurs as the Object case role, it gets the added meaning of being exhaustively affected by the action denoted by the verb as seen in . No such additional meaning is observed in sentences in which the element has been used as a Location case role. How NPs can be assigned either Agent or Experiencer case roles: Evidence found from the meaning distinctions of volition. Example | ra:ju pa:thavannu maretidda:ne Raju lesson has-forgotten "Raju has forgotten the lesson." 3b) ||ra:juvige pa:tha maretide Raju lesson forgotten "The lesson has been forgotten by Raju." Although both sentences indicate the same event, the meaning difference is due to the fact that in Raju, occurring as the Agent, is considered to have acted volitionally, and is hence held responsible for that event, whereas in he, being an Experiencer, is involved in that event only non-volitionally, and hence one does not hold him responsible for it. From the two examples shown above, multiple case roles are available. Different case roles can be assigned to the deep structure representation to give a different semantic meaning. Relating case roles to morphological case and structural CaseSemantic vs. morphologicalThe semantic category of case is related to morphological case. Morphological case reflects the ranking of arguments, while semantic case encodes a semantic relation between the DP and the governing head. Morphological case is typical of complements and is licensed by structural Case. By contrast, semantic case is typical of adjuncts; it is only licensed by the meaning of the head. From the case roles proposed by Fillmore, it was demonstrated that case roles appeared where the morphological cases of dative, genitive or instrumental appeared:
Structural Case is a condition for arguments that originates from a relational head, while morphological case is a property that depends on the NP or DP complement. The relationship between morphological case and structural case is evident in how morphological case is subject to case agreement whereby the morphological case appearing on a DP must be licensed by the syntactic context of the DP. In much of the transformational grammar literature, morphological cases are viewed as determined by the syntactic configuration. In particular, the accusative case is assigned through a structural relation between the verbal head and its complement. For example, the direct complement of a verb is assigned accusative, irrespective of any other properties that it might have. It must be acknowledged that it is not the accusative alone that is structural, rather the specifier of a NP is in the genitive in many languages, and so is the direct object of a nominalized verb. Morphological caseLinguistic typology of morphological caseCase can be further divided into two categories: grammatical cases and semantic cases. Examples of grammatical cases are nominative case, accusative, dative, and ergative. These typically code core grammatical relations which are semantically dependent on the verb, such as subject and object. Semantic cases are instrumental, comitative, and locative cases. These are semantically richer and less dependent on the verb. There exists cases, such as dative, that are borderline between these two categories, having both semantic and grammatical case features.Eight commonly seen cases :
It has been suggested that the lexical case associated with agents is the ergative languages that allow and 2) those that prohibit ergative subjects in intransitive clauses. There can be a distinction made between ergative and accusative type languages with respect to the type of subject and object markings they will display. This distinction is characterized by the type of clauses a language allows, such as ergative, absolutive, accusative, and nominative. The distinction made between language type and clause type is illustrated in the table below:
Case assignment The typological differences among languages has been shown to be the consequence of differences in whether ergative, dative, or other individual case assignment is optional or obligatory in a language. To represent that a particular case is a lexical case, it must be shown that it is theta related. This means that lexical case cannot be assigned to a noun phrase by a verb unless the verb also assigns a theta role to that same noun phrase. Not all lexical cases have the same associated theta role. For example, the dative case and the goal/experiencer theta role; not all, and not only, noun phrases with the theta-role goal/experiencer get marked with the dative case. The class of verbs that mark their subjects with a particular lexical dative case is similar across languages, but the inclusion in this verb class is not completely predictable. Lexical cases such as nominative usually mark predictable theta roles, but there is a substantial amount of characteristic behaviour involved indicating that a verb's ability to assign a lexical case to one of its arguments must be specified in that verb's lexical entry. Once a verb marks its subject with a given lexical case, such as nominative, that verb cannot assign structural accusative case to its object. Morphologically overt case assignment In languages with case marking of explicit morphology, any nominal that is morphologically capable of showing case morphology is obligated to do so. This observation can be stated explicitly by what is referred to as the Case Filter. Case Filter can be further described as being an abstract Case hypothesis that stipulates all lexical noun phrases are assigned a specific Case regardless if this Case manifests at the surface level: If this lexical NP does not have a Case marking at surface structure, then the sentence that contains it is considered ungrammatical. Case theory includes an inventory of structural Case and a series of lexical cases that are assigned at the level of the deep structure in conjunction with theta role assignment. Languages such as Russian and Japanese, among others, follow a similar mechanism of case assignment as that seen in Latin. Russian is like Latin, in that it does have genitive and dative case that is assigned by the N and A. In Russian for example, most nouns show overt case morphology as does Latin, but there is also a productive class of indeclinable nouns. These indeclinable nouns are not able to receive case morphology. Examples from languages exhibiting morphologically overt case marking indicate that there are rules of case assignment present in the grammar of a language. To account for this, rules can be generated as support. For example, support accounting for accusative case in Latin-type case marked languages could be presented as:
Application of these accusative case assignment rules can be seen in the following examples:
For the morphologically rich case language of Latin, there are seven cases for Latin Nouns. The following table demonstrates Latin case morphology assignment for a masculine noun somnus meaning 'sleep'.
|